Régis Vaillancourt, Cindy N Giby, Bradley P Murphy, Annie Pouliot, Anne TrinneerABSTRACT
Background
Low health literacy and high medication burden in the older adult population are contributing factors to the misunderstanding of medication instructions, leading to an increased risk of poor adherence and adverse events in this group of patients.
Objective
To evaluate the ability of older adults to recall the meaning of 13 pharmaceutical pictograms 4 weeks after receipt of feedback on pictogram meaning.
Methods
Older adults (aged 65 or older) were recruited from one community pharmacy in Canada. One-on-one structured interviews were conducted to assess the comprehensibility of 13 pharmaceutical pictograms from the International Pharmaceutical Federation’s database of pictograms. Each participant was then told the meaning of each pictogram. Recall was assessed 4 weeks later.
Results
A total of 58 participants met the inclusion criteria and agreed to participate. The number of pictograms meeting the ISO threshold for comprehensibility of symbols increased from 10 at the initial comprehensibility assessment to 13 at the recall assessment. Analysis of demographic data showed no associations between initial comprehensibility of the pictograms and age, sex, education level, or number of medications taken.
Conclusions
The results of this study indicate that after being informed of the meaning of pharmaceutical pictograms, older adults were able to recall the pictogram meanings for at least 4 weeks.
KEYWORDS: pharmaceutical pictograms, older adults, recall, and comprehensibility
RÉSUMÉ
Contexte
Les faibles connaissances en matière de santé des personnes âgées et le lourd fardeau des médicaments qui pèse sur elles sont des facteurs qui contribuent à l’incompréhension des instructions relatives à l’administration des médicaments, ce qui entraîne un risque plus élevé de mauvaise adhésion au traitement et d’événements indésirables dans ce groupe de patients.
Objectif
Évaluer la capacité des adultes plus âgés à se souvenir du sens des 13 pictogrammes pharmaceutiques, quatre semaines après avoir été informés de leur sens.
Méthodes
Les adultes plus âgés (65 ans et au-delà) ont été recrutés dans une pharmacie communautaire du Canada. Des entrevues structurées en tête-à-tête ont été menées pour évaluer l’intelligibilité de 13 pictogrammes pharmaceutiques extraits de la base de données de la Fédération internationale pharmaceutique. Le sens de chaque pictogramme a ensuite été communiqué à chaque participant et, quatre semaines plus tard, leur capacité à s’en souvenir a été évaluée.
Résultats
Cinquante-huit participants répondaient au critère d’inclusion et ont accepté de participer à l’étude. Le nombre de pictogrammes répondant au seuil ISO d’intelligibilité des symboles est passé de 10 (au moment de l’évaluation d’intelligibilité initiale) à 13 (au moment de l’évaluation du rappel). L’analyse des données démographiques n’a indiqué aucune association entre l’intelligibilité initiale des pictogrammes et l’âge, le sexe, le niveau de formation ou le nombre de médicaments que prenaient ces personnes.
Conclusions
Les résultats de cette étude indiquent qu’après avoir été informés du sens des pictogrammes pharmaceutiques, les aînés étaient en mesure de s’en souvenir pendant au moins quatre semaines.
MOTS CLÉS: pictogrammes pharmaceutiques, aînés, rappel et intelligibilité
Many older adults take numerous medications. Individuals 65 years of age and older account for approximately 15% of the Canadian population, yet they are responsible for nearly 40% of all spending on prescribed medications.1 In 2012, nearly two-thirds of older Canadian adults using public drug programs had claims for 5 or more drug classes.1 Older adults are also more likely than younger people to have limited health literacy.2–6 Health literacy is important in the effective management of chronic disease because it affects the ability to understand the nature of one’s medical condition7 and the ability to perform self-care, especially among older adults.8,9 This combination of lower health literacy and high prescription drug use likely contributes to the fact that older adults are at high risk for adverse drug events10 and for misinterpreting medication instructions.11,12 Misunderstanding of medication instructions may lead to poor adherence13,14 and medication errors.15 Cognitive aging further contributes to this process, which poses an additional risk for non-adherence and adverse events.13 Therefore, it is important to develop tools to help older adults to understand the instructions for taking their medications. Although adherence with medication therapy is multifactorial,16 improved comprehension may improve adherence and clinical outcomes, which will in turn reduce health care costs.17,18
Pharmacists generally provide counselling about prescription medications just once, when a prescription is initially filled,19 even though some medications are taken for many months or virtually indefinitely. During these consultations, information is provided verbally and/or in written form. Medical information presented verbally may not be well retained.20,21 In addition, much of this written material is not adapted to match the patient’s education level, and the documentation can be long and complex,15,22,23 which may be challenging, especially for older adults.24 Nonetheless, numerous reviews have demonstrated that the communication of medication information by pharmacists can be very effective. Pharmacist-led educational interventions have improved adherence to medication in depression,25 type 2 diabetes,26 and chronic obstructive pulmonary disease,27 and have improved clinical outcomes in patients with type 2 diabetes28 and hypertension. 29 The association between medication adherence and health services utilization and cost is well established, with even moderate improvement in adherence being associated with reductions in utilization and cost.17,18 Thus, finding effective interventions to improve adherence is worth the effort.
One step toward improving medication adherence is to improve patients’ understanding of medication instructions. Implementation of pictograms depicting key counselling points during medication consultations may improve comprehension and retention of these key points. Pictograms, when added to patient information, represent an intervention that has been shown to improve patient comprehension of health information generally30,31 and medication information more specifically.32,33 Many studies of pharmaceutical pictograms have been conducted in various populations. Pharmaceutical pictograms have been tested for their ability to improve understanding and recall of medication instructions in individuals with low literacy,31,34 those taking long-term medications,32,35–56 older adults,32,38 women,39 and adults.40,41 Results have been mixed. The variation in these results may be explained, at least in part, by whether the pictograms were first demonstrated to be comprehensible in the population of interest.42
We know from numerous published studies on the comprehensibility of pharmaceutical pictograms that at least a few pictograms in each trial will not be understood by participants and that the extent of pictogram comprehensibility depends greatly on the population in which they are tested.42 Researchers have tested pharmaceutical pictograms for comprehensibility in individual ethnic, cultural, and language groups43–48; in older adults49–51; in patients with low literacy52–56; in children and youth57,58; and in adults.59–63 A recent review of patient involvement in pictogram design indicated that studies using an iterative process of design and redesign based on patient feedback tend to produce pictograms that are well understood.42
The purpose of this study was to evaluate the ability of older adults to understand and recall the meaning of pharmaceutical pictograms used to convey key medication counselling points. Recall was measured after a 1-month (4-week) interval because this is a typical refill period for prescription medications. Recall was assessed because of the possibility that some pharmaceutical pictograms may not be recognizable, no matter how often they are redesigned. It may be possible, however, that older adults will remember the meaning of a pictogram after being informed of its meaning.
The 13 pictograms used in this study were taken from the International Pharmaceutical Federation (FIP) database (https://www.fipfoundation.org/pictogram-project/usingpictograms/). They depict key counselling points related to indications, side effects, routes and frequencies of administration, and precautions. All of these pictograms were developed using a patient-centred approach, with participants drawn from the general population.64 Thus, they were not initially developed specifically for use in older adults; however, they were subsequently tested in a sample of older adults51 using the International Organization for Standardization (ISO) criteria for development of public information symbols.65 According to the ISO 9186-1 standard,65 in order to be considered comprehensible, the meaning of a pictogram must be correctly understood by at least 66.7% of participants. In the initial study with older adults, pictograms that were not well understood were modified by a graphic designer on the basis of participants’ suggestions, when available, and were then retested.51 Despite redesign, 47 pictograms (out of 76) remained poorly understood in this sample of older adults.51 This result not only highlighted the importance of testing pictograms for comprehensibility among older adults, but also suggested the importance of including a recall phase in the experimental design. The meaning of some pictograms may never be “guessable” by some populations, but if participants can recall the meaning after it has been provided, this suggests that the pictogram may be able to convey its intended meaning when paired with information about its meaning. For the current study, we chose 13 pictograms from the previous study with older adults,51 representing medication instructions that we considered to be most useful for older adults. Notably, 10 of these 13 pictograms did not meet the 66.7% threshold when initially tested with older adults.51
Individuals aged 65 years or older who had prescriptions for at least 3 medications were recruited from a single community pharmacy in Ottawa, Ontario, Canada. Potential participants were excluded if they resided in an assisted-living facility, had self-declared functional impairment (e.g., blindness), or were taking a medication for cognitive impairment (e.g., dementia). Visual acuity was not assessed. However, it is likely that cognitive impairment would affect the results of any test of pictogram comprehensibility. Therefore, the Mini-Cog,66 a 3-item test of cognitive abilities that is as sensitive and specific in testing for dementia as the Mini–Mental State Exam and the Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument, was administered to all potential participants. A Mini-Cog score of less than 3 (out of 5) indicates impaired cognitive status.66 Only participants with a score of 4 or 5 were included in the present study. To validate pictograms for use with older adults who have cognitive impairment, it would be necessary to select a sample consisting entirely of participants with cognitive impairment; however, this was not the purpose of the current study.
Potential participants who did not meet the inclusion criteria or who did not agree to participate continued to receive services as usual in the pharmacy.
Demographic data collected were sex, age, education level, language spoken at home, and number of long-term medications being taken.
The comprehensibility of the pictograms was determined by an assessment of transparency. The concept of transparency refers to how easily the meaning of a symbol can be guessed when the referent is not present.67 Participants’ responses on transparency testing were scored as correct or incorrect by 2 independent raters (B.P.M. and A.P.). Any disagreements among the raters were discussed with a third person, and a decision on scoring was reached by consensus.
When a potentially eligible participant came to the pharmacy to fill a prescription, a pharmacist or pharmacy technician asked whether he or she was interested in participating in the study. A fully bilingual (English and French) pharmacy technician conducted one-on-one structured interviews with participants, both during the initial assessment and at follow-up. The ability to conduct these interviews in either English or French was important because almost 9% of the population of Ottawa and surrounding area speak only French,68 and we did not wish to exclude such a large proportion of the population. During the initial assessment, the interviewer first administered the Mini-Cog test to screen for cognitive impairment, as described above. Only participants who passed the Mini-Cog test were asked to complete the remainder of the assessment.
The 13 pictograms, printed on 25-cm2 cue cards, were shuffled before each session and presented sequentially. For each pictogram, the participant was asked what he or she thought the pictogram meant in the context of taking medication. The responses were transcribed verbatim by the interviewer. Immediately after presenting all 13 pictograms, the interviewer then informed the participant of the intended meaning of each pictogram. The demographic questionnaire was administered at the end of this interview.
Four weeks later, the participants were invited (via telephone call from a pharmacy technician) to complete the recall assessment. During the recall assessment, which was conducted in person in the pharmacy, the identical procedure was followed, with the technician presenting the pictograms and asking the participant what he or she thought each pictogram meant in the context of taking medication. No other assessments or questionnaires were administered at the recall assessment.
Approval for this study was obtained from the Research Ethics Board of the Children’s Hospital of Eastern Ontario. All participants provided written consent to participate in the research process. Each participant received a $10 gift card redeemable at the pharmacy.
All analyses were conducted with IBM SPSS Statistics version 24.0 (IBM Corp, Armonk, New York). Categorical variables were analyzed using frequencies and percentages. Normally distributed continuous variables were summarized using means and standard deviations (SDs). McNemar tests were performed to compare the number of participants who correctly understood the meaning of each pictogram during transparency testing with the number who correctly recalled the meaning 4 weeks later. A repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to determine whether there was a difference between participants’ comprehension of all pictograms before and after being told the meanings. Subgroup analyses were conducted using χ2 analyses with the Fisher exact test to identify differences in pictogram comprehensibility in relation to highest level of education completed (middle/high school versus college, university, or postgraduate), sex, Mini-Cog test score (4 versus 5), and number of long-term medications being taken (3 or 4 versus 5 or more). Similarly, one-way ANOVAs were conducted to test for differences in pictogram comprehensibility by age. Given the large number of subanalyses carried out (n = 65), the threshold p value for significance in these analyses was set at 0.05/65 or 0.0007.
A total of 58 participants met the inclusion criteria and agreed to participate. This sample size was considered adequate because the ISO standard65 states that pictograms should be tested with a minimum sample of 50 participants. Of the 58 participants who met the inclusion criteria and agreed to participate, 30 were women, 25 were men, and sex was not reported for 3 participants (Table 1). The mean age of participants was 74.2 (SD 6.1), with 26 (45%) being 75 years or older. There was no age difference between men (mean 74.8, SD 7.0) and women (mean 74.2, SD 5.5) (t(53) = 0.38, p = 0.71). Of those who provided information about their level of education, 98% (52/53) had completed at least high school. The mean number of prescription medications being taken by participants was 4.9 (SD 6.1), with 28% of participants taking 6 or more prescription medications. All 58 participants completed both the initial interview (transparency assessment) and the recall assessment.
Table 1 Demographic Characteristics of Participants
Of the 13 pictograms tested in this study, 10 reached the ISO standard for comprehensibility, with at least 66.7% of participants understanding the meaning during the transparency assessment, that is, upon initial presentation before being told the intended meaning (Table 2). The pictograms for “confusion” (52%), “diarrhea” (57%), and “take in the morning” (48%) did not meet the ISO comprehensibility threshold (Table 2). These 3 pictograms were also among those that did not meet the threshold in the previous study with older adults.51
Table 2 Comprehensibility and Recall Scores
During the recall assessment, 4 weeks after participants were told the meaning of the pictograms, all 13 pictograms reached the ISO standard for comprehensibility. Statistically significant differences in the proportions of participants comprehending the pictograms between the transparency and recall assessments were observed for 9 pictograms: “tremors”, “confusion”, “dizzy when getting up”, “nausea”, “diarrhea”, “shake well”, “do not crush”, “take in the morning”, and “seek medical assistance” (Table 2).
As an additional test of whether comprehension of the pictograms was better at the recall assessment than at the transparency assessment, a repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted, comparing the total number of pictograms understood correctly by each participant at the recall assessment with the total number understood at the transparency assessment. The result was statistically significant (Wilks λ = 0.38, F[1,57] = 93.41, p < 0.001), with the average number of pictograms understood correctly being higher at the recall assessment (mean 12.6, SD 0.8), than at transparency assessment (mean 9.9, SD 2.3).
Our analyses indicated no statistically significant associations between pictogram comprehensibility and age, education level, sex, number of prescription medications, or Mini-Cog score (Table 3).
Table 3 Subanalysis of Initial Pictogram Comprehensibility in Relation to Demographic Characteristics
In this study, older adults could correctly recall the meaning of 13 pharmaceutical pictograms 4 weeks after initial assessment, even if they initially did not correctly understand the meaning of the pictogram. For 9 of the 13 pictograms tested—“tremors”, “confusion”, “dizzy when getting up”, “nausea”, “diarrhea”, “shake well”, “do not crush”, “take in the morning”, and “seek medical assistance”—more participants correctly stated the meaning at the recall assessment than at the initial presentation. In a previous study with older adult participants,51 none of these pictograms met the ISO standard of 66.7% of participants being able to guess their meaning, but all participants in the current study met the standard at the recall assessment.
The 3 pictograms that met the ISO threshold for comprehensibility in the previous study with older adults study51 (“take 1 tablet by mouth”, “headache”, and “do not mix with alcohol”) also did so in the transparency assessment of the current study. The pictograms for “confusion”, “diarrhea”, and “take in the morning” did not meet the ISO threshold in either the previous study51 or the transparency assessment of the current study. In contrast to these similarities in results, 7 pictograms that were not understood by older adults previously51 (“tremors”, “fatigue”, “nausea”, “shake well”, “do not crush”, “seek medical assistance”, and “dizzy when getting up”) were guessed correctly by more than 66.7% of participants in the current study. There are some differences in the study samples that may explain why participants in the current study were able to understand the meaning of more of the pictograms. The mean age of participants in the current sample was 5 years younger than that of the sample in the previous study.51 In addition, there was no screening for cognitive capacity in the previous study.51 Thus, it is possible that the higher mean age in the previous study51 was associated with age-related decline in cognitive capacity, which might have affected pictogram interpretability. Also, most participants in the previous study had fewer than 12 years of education, whereas the majority of the current sample had more than a high school education. It is likely that a sample with fewer years of education would also have lower health literacy. To understand the meaning of a pictogram within the context of taking medications, a person must draw upon health-related knowledge. Thus, it may be that participants in the current study had more knowledge upon which to draw when describing what they thought each pictogram meant in the context of taking medication.
The results of this study demonstrate the importance of counselling older adult patients to ensure they understand the meaning of the pharmaceutical pictograms that accompany their prescription medications. Del Re and others31 conducted a literature review to evaluate the effectiveness of pictograms to improve patients’ recall of medication safety instructions. They speculated that older adults have increased difficulty in recalling pictograms because of an unclear understanding of the information presented.31 These authors proposed that special consideration be given to older adults and that indeed all patients should be counselled when pictograms are used in a health care setting. Their recommendation reflects current standards set by the FIP, which state that “graphic symbols for patient instruction should not be used alone but should always be combined with written instructions”. 69 The importance of using pictograms together with verbal or written information has been documented in other studies31,54,70 and has been considered from a theoretical stand-point in the dual coding theory proposed by Paivio.71 This author stated that information is processed by verbal and nonverbal coding systems.71 Furthermore, pictures or images trigger the activation of both systems to a greater extent than words alone, leading to improved recall of information.71 By extension, the recruitment of multiple senses through the use of verbal and written instructions together with pictograms will likely lead to improved recall.
Among the limitations of the current study is the fact that we did not assess participants’ visual acuity. It is possible that some participants did not understand certain of the pictograms because of vision problems. In addition, we did not assess health literacy. Given that pictograms are often implemented to help people with low levels of health literacy to better understand their medication administration instructions, it will be important to investigate how well older adults with low health literacy understand these pictograms and recall their meanings. Potential participants with cognitive impairment were excluded from the current study. Thus, another limitation of the study is that the results can be generalized only to older adults without cognitive impairment.
Given that the intended meaning of all 13 pictograms included in this study could be recalled by at least 66.7% of participants after 4 weeks, we recommend that future research in the development of pictograms with older adults should assess recall of pictogram meaning and not rely on transparency assessment alone. Given the low health literacy levels noted among older adults in other studies,2–5 it may not always be possible for this age group to understand the meaning of pharmaceutical pictograms without explanation. They may, however, be able to recall pictogram meanings once they have been explained.
It would also be interesting to know whether use of these pictograms can increase adherence to medication regimens among older adults. Any future research on the effect of these pictograms on medication adherence among older adults should implement recently published guidelines for conducting effective research on medication adherence.72
Four weeks after being informed of the intended meanings of pictograms depicting medication instructions, older adults were able to recall the pictogram meanings. Thus, this set of pictograms may be used in practice with older adults to convey key counselling points, in combination with verbal and written instructions. As stated by FIP, “graphic symbols for patient instruction should not be used alone but should always be combined with written instructions”.69
1 Proulx J, Hunt J. Drug use among seniors on public drug programs in Canada, 2012. Healthc Q. 2015;18(1):11–3.
2 The full Canadian report of the international adult literacy and skills survey. Ottawa (ON): Statistics Canada; 2005.
3 Kobayashi LC, Wardle J, Wolf MS, Von Wagner C. Aging and functional health literacy: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2016;71(3):445–57.
4 Von Wagner C, Knight K, Steptoe A, Wardle J. Functional health literacy and health-promoting behaviour in a national sample of British adults. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2007;61(12):1086–90.
5 Paasche-Orlow MK, Parker RM, Gazmararian JA, Nielsen-Bohlman LT, Rudd RR. The prevalence of limited health literacy. J Gen Intern Med. 2005;20(2):175–84.
6 Zamora H, Clingerman EM. Health literacy among older adults: a systematic literature review. J Gerontol Nurs. 2011;37(10):41–51.
7 Du S, Zhou Y, Fu C, Wang Y, Du X, Xie R. Health literacy and health outcomes in hypertension: an integrative review. Int J Nurs Sci. 2018;5(3): 301–9.
8 Cho YI, Lee SYD, Arozullah AM, Crittenden KS. Effects of health literacy on health status and health service utilization amongst the elderly. Soc Sci Med. 2008;66(8):1809–16.
9 Hanchate AD, Ash AS, Gazmararian JA, Wolf MS, Paasche-Orlow MK. The Demographic Assessment for Health Literacy (DAHL): a new tool for estimating associations between health literacy and outcomes in national surveys. J Gen Intern Med. 2008;23(10):1561–6.
10 Oscanoa TJ, Lizaraso F, Carvajal A. Hospital admissions due to adverse drug reactions in the elderly. A meta-analysis. Eur J Clin Pharmacol. 2017;73(6):759–70.
11 Davis TC, Wolf MS, Bass PF 3rd, Thompson JA, Tilson HH, Neuberger M, et al. Literacy and misunderstanding prescription drug labels. Ann Intern Med. 2006;145(12):887–94.
12 Maher RL Jr, Hanlon JT, Hajjar ER. Clinical consequences of polypharmacy in elderly. Expert Opin Drug Saf. 2014;13(1):57–65.
13 Campbell NL, Boustani MA, Skopelja EN, Gao S, Unverzagt FW, Murray MD. Medication adherence in older adults with cognitive impairment: a systematic evidence-based review. Am J Geriatr Pharmacother. 2012; 10(3):165–77.
14 Wolf MS, Feinglass J, Thompson J, Baker DW. In search of ‘low health literacy’: threshold vs. gradient effect of literacy on health status and mortality. Soc Sci Med. 2010;70(9):1335–41.
15 Wolf MS, Davis TC, Shrank W, Rapp DN, Bass PF, Connor UM, et al. To err is human: patient misinterpretations of prescription drug label instructions. Patient Educ Couns. 2007;67(3):293–300.
16 Brown MT, Bussell JK. Medication adherence: WHO cares? Mayo Clin Proc. 2011;86(4):304–14.
17 Sokol MC, McGuigan KA, Verbrugge RR, Epstein RS. Impact of medication adherence on hospitalization risk and healthcare cost. Med Care. 2005; 43(6):521–30.
18 Roebuck MC, Dougherty JS. Impact of medication adherence on hospitalization in Medicaid. Value Health. 2016;19(3):A9.
19 Puspitasari HP, Aslani P, Krass I. A review of counseling practices on prescription medicines in community pharmacies. Res Soc Adm Pharm. 2009; 5(3):197–210.
20 Rajda C, George NM. The effect of education and literacy levels on health outcomes of the elderly. J Nurse Pract. 2009;5(2):115–9.
21 Kessels RPC. Patients’ memory for medical information. J R Soc Med.2003; 96(5):219–22.
22 Pires C, Vigário M, Cavaco A. Readability of medicinal package leaflets: a systematic review. Rev Saude Publica. 2015;49:4.
23 Wolf MS, King J, Wilson EAH, Curtis LM, Bailey SC, Dhuig J, et al. Usability of FDA-approved medication guides. J Gen Intern Med.2012; 27(12):1714–20.
24 Liu F, Abdul-Hussain S, Mahboob S, Rai V, Kostrzewski A. How useful are medication patient information leaflets to older adults? A content, readability and layout analysis. Int J Clin Pharm. 2014;36(4):827–34.
25 Readdean KC, Heuer AJ, Parrott JS. Effect of pharmacist intervention on improving antidepressant medication adherence and depression symptomology: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Res Soc Adm Pharm. 2018; 14(4);321–31.
26 van Eikenhorst L, Taxis K, van Dijk L, de Gier H. Pharmacist-led self-management interventions to improve diabetes outcomes. A systematic literature review and meta-analysis. Front Pharmacol. 2017;8:891.
27 Hesso I, Gebara SN, Kayyali R. Impact of community pharmacists in COPD management: inhalation technique and medication adherence. Respir Med. 2016;118:22–30.
28 Bukhsh A, Khan TM, Lee SWH, Lee LH, Chang KG, Goh BH. Efficacy of pharmacist based diabetes educational interventions on clinical outcomes of adults with type 2 diabetes mellitus: a network meta-analysis. Front Pharmacol. 2018;9:339.
29 Cheema E, Sutcliffe P, Singer DRJ. The impact of interventions by pharmacists in community pharmacies on control of hypertension: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 2014;78(6):1238–47.
30 Peregrin T. Picture this: visual cues enhance health education messages for people with low literacy skills. J Am Diet Assoc. 2010;110(4):500,502–5.
31 Del Re L, Vaillancourt R, Villarreal G, Pouliot A. Pictograms: can they help patients recall medication safety instructions? Visible Lang. 2016; 50(1):127–51.
32 Ng AWY, Chan AHS, Ho VWS. Comprehension by older people of medication information with or without supplementary pharmaceutical pictograms. Appl Ergon. 2017;58:167–75.
33 Mullen RJ, Duhig J, Russell A, Scarazzini L, Lievano F, Wolf MS. Best-practices for the design and development of prescription medication information: a systematic review. Patient Educ Couns. 2018;101(8):1351–67.
34 Dowse R, Ehlers M. Medicine labels incorporating pictograms: do they influence understanding and adherence? Patient Educ Couns. 2005;58(1): 63–70.
35 Chan HK, Hassali MA. Modified labels for long-term medications: influences on adherence, comprehension and preferences in Malaysia. Int J Clin Pharm. 2014;36(5):904–13.
36 Mansoor L, Dowse R. Written medicines information for South African HIV/AIDS patients: does it enhance understanding of co-trimoxazole therapy? Health Educ Res. 2007;22(1):37–48.
37 Wilby K, Marra CA, da Silva JH, Grubisic M, Harvard S, Lynd LD. Randomized controlled trial evaluating pictogram augmentation of HIV medication information. Ann Pharmacother. 2011;45(11):1378–83.
38 Malhotra R, Bautista MAC, Tan NC, Tang WE, Tay S, Tan S, et al. Bilingual text with or without pictograms improves elderly Singaporeans’ understanding of prescription medication labels. Gerontologist. 2019;59(2):378–90.
39 You WB, Grobman W, Davis T, Curtis LM, Bailey SC, Wolf M. Improving pregnancy drug warnings to promote patient comprehension. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2011;204(4):318.e1–5.
40 Advani AA, Lopez J, Jones J, Patel S. The role of pictograms for enhancement of patient prescription medication information. J Pharm Technol. 2013; 29(1):40–5.
41 Wolf MS, Davis TC, Bass PF, Curtis LM, Lindquist LA, Webb JA, et al. Improving prescription drug warnings to promote patient comprehension. Arch Intern Med. 2010;170(1):50–6.
42 van Beusekom MM, Kerkhoven AH, Bos MJW, Guchelaar HJ, van den Broek JM. The extent and effects of patient involvement in pictogram design for written drug information: a short systematic review. Drug Discov Today. 2018;23(6):1312–8.
43 Sorfleet C, Vaillancourt R, Groves S, Dawson J. Design, development and evaluation of pictographic instructions for medications used during humanitarian missions. Can Pharm J. 2009;142(2):82–8.
44 Zargarzadeh AH, Ahamdi S. Comprehensibility of selected USP pictograms by illiterate and literate Farsi speakers: the first experience in Iran - part I. J Res Med Sci. 2017;22:84.
45 Zargarzadeh AH, Ahamdi S. Comprehensibility of selected United States Pharmacopeia pictograms by illiterate and literate Farsi speakers: the first experience in Iran – part II. J Res Med Sci. 2017;22:101.
46 Barros IMC, Alcântara TS, Mesquita AR, Bispo ML, Rocha CE, Moreira VP, et al. Understanding of pictograms from the United States Pharmacopeia Dispensing Information (USP-DI) among elderly Brazilians. Patient Prefer Adherence. 2014;8:1493–501.
47 Dowse R, Ehlers M. Pictograms for conveying medicine instructions: comprehension in various South African language groups. S Afr J Sci. 2004; 100:687–93.
48 Kassam R, Vaillancourt R, Collins JB. Pictographic instructions for medications: do different cultures interpret them accurately? Int J Pharm Pract. 2004;12(4):199–209.
49 Merks P, SwSieczkowski D, Balcerzak M, Drelich E, Białoszewska K, Cwalina N, et al. The evaluation of pharmaceutical pictograms among elderly patients in community pharmacy settings – a multicenter pilot study. Patient Prefer Adherence. 2018;12:257–66.
50 Vaillancourt R, Pouliot A, Streitenberger K, Hyland S, Thabet P. Pictograms for safer medication management by health care workers. Can J Hosp Pharm. 2016;69(4):286–93.
51 Berthenet M, Vaillancourt R, Pouliot A. Evaluation, modification, and validation of pictograms depicting medication instructions in the elderly. J Health Commun. 2016;21 Suppl 1:27–33.
52 van Beusekom MM, Land-Zandstra AM, Bos MJW, van den Broek JM, Guchelaar HJ. Pharmaceutical pictograms for low-literate patients: understanding, risk of false confidence, and evidence-based design strategies. Patient Educ Couns. 2017;100(5):966–73.
53 Kheir N, Awaisu A, Radoui A, El Badawi A, Jean L, Dowse R. Development and evaluation of pictograms on medication labels for patients with limited literacy skills in a culturally diverse multiethnic population. Res Social Adm Pharm. 2014;10(5):720–30.
54 Joshi Y, Kothiyal P. A pilot study to evaluate pharmaceutical pictograms in a multispecialty hospital at Dehradun. J Young Pharm. 2011;3(2):163–6.
55 Cutler TW, Chuang A, Huynh TD, Witt RG, Branch J, Pon T, et al. A retrospective descriptive analysis of patient adherence to dabigatran at a large academic medical center. J Manag Care Spec Pharm. 2014;20(10):1028–34.
56 Chuang MH, Lin CL, Wang YF, Cham TM. Development of pictographs depicting medication use instructions for low-literacy medical clinic ambulatory patients. J Manag Care Spec Pharm. 2016;16(5):337–45.
57 Korenevsky A, Vaillancourt R, Pouliot A, Revol M, Steed E, Besançon L, et al. How many words does a picture really tell? Cross-sectional descriptive study of pictogram evaluation by youth. Can J Hosp Pharm. 2013;66(4): 219–26.
58 Revol M, Vaillancourt R, Pouliot A. Evaluation, validation, and modification of pictograms depicting potential side effects to medication. J Commun Healthc. 2013;6(2):79–89.
59 Yu B, Willis M, Sun P, Wang J. Crowdsourcing participatory evaluation of medical pictograms using Amazon Mechanical Turk. J Med Internet Res. 2013;15(6):e108.
60 Chan AHS, Chan KWL. Effects of prospective-user factors and sign design features on guessability of pharmaceutical pictograms. Patient Educ Couns. 2013;90(2):268–75.
61 Soares MA. Legibility of USP pictograms by clients of community pharmacies in Portugal. Int J Clin Pharm. 2013;35(1):22–9.
62 Richler M, Vaillancourt R, Celetti SJ, Besançon L, Arun KP, Sebastien F. The use of pictograms to convey health information regarding side effects and/or indications of medications. J Commun Healthc. 2012;5(4):220–6.
63 Knapp P, Raynor DK, Jebar AH, Price SJ. Interpretation of medication pictograms by adults in the UK. Ann Pharmacother. 2005;39(7–8):1227–33.
64 Wilson EAH, Vaillancourt R, Pascuet E, Besançon LJR, Wolf MS. Seeking international consensus in the use of icons for medication instructions. J Commun Heal. 2012;5(1):67–72.
65 Technical Committee ISO/TC 145 Graphical Symbols. ISO 9186-1:2014. Graphical symbols — test methods — part I: method for testing comprehensibility. Geneva (CH): International Organization for Standardization; 2014.
66 Borson S, Scanlan J, Brush M, Vitaliano P, Dokmak A. The Mini-Cog: a cognitive ‘vital signs’ measure for dementia screening in multi-lingual elderly. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2000;15(11):1021–7.
67 Carmeli S, Shen Y. Semantic transparency and translucency in compound blissymbols. Augment Altern Commun. 1998;14(3):171–83.
68 Visual census. 2011 Census. Ottawa (ON): Statistics Canada; 2012.
69 FIP guidelines for the labels of prescribed medicines. The Hague (Netherlands): International Pharmaceutical Federation; 2001.
70 Houts PS, Witmer JT, Egeth HE, Loscalzo MJ, Zabora JR. Using pictographs to enhance recall of spoken medical instructions II. Patient Educ Couns. 2001;43(3):231–42.
71 Paivio A. Imagery and verbal processes. Oxford (UK): Holt, Rinehart and Winston; 1971.
72 De Geest S, Zullig LL, Dunbar-Jacob J, Helmy R, Hughes DA, Wilson IB, et al. ESPACOMP Medication Adherence Reporting Guideline (EMERGE). Ann Intern Med, 2018;169(1):30–5.
Competing interests: None declared. ( Return to Text )
Funding: None received. ( Return to Text )
Canadian Journal of Hospital Pharmacy, VOLUME 72, NUMBER 6, November-December 2019